Dissect-a-Face
Distinctive universal expressions have been identified for joy, sadness, anger, fear, and disgust. Most textbooks agree that these five plus surprise are common to all cultures.
Thirteen years after he published The Origin of Species, Charles Darwin published The Expression of Emotions in Man and Animals, which argued that emotions and their expression are not unique to humans. Darwin devised a number of principles for comparing emotions in humans and other species – serviceable habits, antithesis, direct action of the nervous system. His observations suggest that certain expressions are shared not only across cultures, but also across species as well, and his conclusions continue to generate debate even today. Although quite radical in its day, The Expression of Emotions in Man and Animals was perhaps always destined to become a footnote to Darwin’s other works. By the early twentieth century, the prevailing view was that facial expressions were learned in childhood and were unbound by universal principles. Darwin’s hypothesis remained largely forgotten for decades until a series of publications by psychologist Paul Ekman reignited the debate. Although somewhat controversial in some aspects, Ekman’s cross-cultural research is definitive in concluding that the ability to read and make certain faces is universal and are biologically determined. Distinctive universal expressions have been identified for joy, sadness, anger, fear, and disgust. Most textbooks agree that these five plus surprise are the fundamental universal facial expressions that are common to all cultures. However, there is a growing body of evidence that expressions of interest and contempt may also be universally shared. The very fact that these hypotheses have generated such debate underlines how difficult it is to disentangle the social and biological aspects integral to reading faces. Clearly, the ability to read expressions is integral to our ability to socialize. When we lose this ability, as is the case in many individuals with autism, this can have a devastating impact on our ability to form social bonds. Use the Dissect-a-Face tool on the next page to explore the implicit clues which most use to read expressions. You can also recreate new expressions by combining different eyes and mouths.
face, facial expressions, emotions, reading faces, joy, sadness, anger, fear, surprise, disgust, darwin, expression of emotions, man, animals
- ID: 866
- Source: DNALC.G2C
Related Content
867. Reading Faces
The following tool was designed to test how accurately you can determine emotional expressions.
1354. Autism (lesson)
Students learn about the symptoms associated with autism, explore the nature of 'normal' behavior, and can design a quasi-experiment to test a hypothesis about autism.
1288. Neuroimaging and Autism
Neuroimaging studies of autism highlight a dysfunctional mirror neuron system, particularly in an area called the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex.
2360. Bipolar Disorder, Autism and Facial Expressions
Doctor Ellen Leibenluft explains that although individuals with bipolar disorder can have trouble interpreting emotional expressions, this is much more subtle than in autism.
872. Autism Neuropathology
Autism is not associated with any single deficit in the brain.
2074. The amygdala and depression
Professor Wayne Drevets outlines the amygdala's importance to the neurobiology of depression. He concludes that stimulation of the amygdala can elicit depression-like emotional experiences.
852. Mirror Neurons and Empathy
Empathy, research indicates, is made possible by a special group of nerve cells called mirror neurons.
822. The Amygdala, the Body's Alarm Circuit
The amygdala controls autonomic responses associated with fear, arousal, and emotional stimulation and has been linked to anxiety disorder and social phobias.
2132. Deep brain stimulation - success!
Professor Helen Mayberg describes the incredible transformation in some patients following deep brain stimulation to treat depression.
825. A Brain Built for Fair Play
A new theory of the neuroscientific basis for the human instinct for fair play.